Beautiful article about electroconvulsive therapy from family therapist Merrill Weiner (essential reading for those of us who spent more of their formative years listening to her big brother’s records than is strictly healthy).
BrainWonk
A blog about brains
Thursday 16 April 2015
Wednesday 5 December 2012
Genetic overlap between autism, schizophrenia and bipolar disorder
Aha...good sense from Carroll & Owen in 2009 [1]:
"It is clear that much future work is required and equally clear that this should not be constrained by current categorical diagnostic systems. Such studies should explore the relationship of genes and other biological variables to dimensional measures of key domains of psychopathology across current diagnostic categories. We have previously argued the need to undertake such endeavors across the functional psychoses of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. However, recent data point to the need to consider a broader clinical spectrum that includes also autism and mental retardation/cognitive impairment
No reason to think that DSM-5 changes anything here.
Note to self: must start posting here more to avoid need for embarrassing new year's resolutions.
[1] Carroll LS, Owen MJ (2009). Genetic overlap between autism, schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Genome Medicine 1:102.
http://genomemedicine.com/content/1/10/102
Friday 10 August 2012
DSM, diagnose thyself
There’s an eye-popping synapse-tingling connectome-reconfiguring passage in The Psychopath Test, the new book from Jon Ronson (the man who wrote The Men Who Stare at Goats), when he interviews Robert Spitzer, the architect of the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM III), and Allen Frances, chief editor of DSM IV.
DSM-III, Ronson explains, was in part a reaction to the audacious Rosenhan experiment when eight healthy American volunteers presented themselves to psychiatrists throughout the United States. The volunteers gave false names and jobs, but otherwise followed instructions to answer every question the psychiatrists posed truthfully except in one respect: they were to report that they could hear an indistinct disembodied voice saying something that sounded like “hollow” or “empty” or “thud”. All eight were admitted to psychiatric wards where they told staff that the one abnormal symptom, the hallucinated voice, had stopped. All eight were diagnosed insane, given powerful drugs, and released 7 to 52 days later, and only when they admitted insanity. Reassuringly, when the findings were published in the journal Science, the American psychiatric profession experienced a collective breakdown and was sucked into a vortex of self-doubt. Spitzer, a progressive psychiatrist who had argued successfully for the removal of homosexuality as a mental disorder, began the healing process by reforming the DSM, replacing old discredited clinical judgement with new rigorous repeatable road-tested diagnostic checklists. The enterprise seems to have come naturally - perhaps too naturally - to Spitzer who classes himself as one of nature’s categorizers. Unfortunately, by all accounts the undertaking didn’t quite reach the heights of clinical excellence a layperson might naively hope for: “the psychiatrists Spitzer invited [to DSM-III editorial meetings] would yell over each other. The person with the loudest voice tended to get taken the most seriously.... 'Of course we didn’t take minutes’ says Spitzer, 'We barely had a typewriter'”.
Allen Frances, Spitzer’s successor, is delightfully candid about the results: “It’s very easy to set off a false epidemic in psychiatry and we inadvertently contributed to three that are ongoing now: autism, attention deficit and childhood bipolar. With autism it was mostly adding Asperger’s...the rates of autistic disorder in children went from less than one in two thousand to more than one in one hundred. Many kids who would have been called eccentric , different were suddenly labelled autistic”.
In a recent op-ed piece in the New York Times about the forthcoming DSM 5 his criticism is even sharper: “ DSM 5 promises to be a disaster... it will introduce many new and unproven diagnoses that will medicalize normality and result in a glut of unnecessary and harmful drug prescription.”
Can we, should we do better than the DSM? Part of the problem with the DSM’s diagnostic checklists is surely, as Allen Frances suggests, that unusual behaviours, which are in the tails of the bell-curve of normal behaviour, are labelled as disorders. When the behaviour to which the label is attached (idiosyncratic language, preoccupations unusual in intensity or focus, whatever) is not itself a cause of suffering, this seems hugely wrong-headed. Another problem is that diverse conditions that may have nothing in common biologically and which have quite distinct prognoses, and possibly respond very differently to interventions, are being lumped together as the same condition. What’s more, completely different diagnostic criteria may have overlapping symptoms, calling into question the value of the distinct diagnoses.
There’s an intriguing article in Neuron by Joshua Buckholtz and Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg that reckons that by using some deliciously clever ideas from network theory we might be able to do better than current practice. The paper argues that, as much as it must irk the natural born categorizer, many mental disorders probably don’t divide neatly into discrete types in the way that butterflies and beetles do. What’s more, mistakenly grouping them together could be actively harmful: “Many in the field believe that the notion of discrete, categorical mental disorders, originally articulated by the Research Diagnostic Criteria and reified in the DSM-III and DSM-IV, is so far removed from biological reality that it actually impedes clinically useful scientific discovery. ...Though originally intended to be ‘merely’ reliable operationalizations of clinical phenomena, over time, these categorical classifications came to be treated as though they were natural kinds — inherently meaningful, ontologically (i.e., biologically) valid taxons. This has produced the assumption that each DSM-defined disorder is ‘‘real’’—a distinct, independent entity with a unique set of causal factors and pathophysiological processes.”
The authors argue that this view that all mental disorders fall into distinct biologically meaningfully categories is not supported by evidence: “comorbidity between mental disorders is the rule rather than the exception...covariation among psychiatric diagnoses is so prevalent, and so extensive, that it alone belies the artificial nature of phenomenologically based categorical classification.”
Part of the solution, they argue, may be to reverse engineer the wiring map (or connectome) of the brain, and look for anomalies, similar patterns of dodgy wiring that could underly may different mental disorders. This is possible, it turns out, by photographing blood oxygen levels in different parts of the brain over time in an MRI scanner, and analysing which bits light up and in what order. With this information and some heavy-duty computational statistics, it’s possible to deduce something about the strength of the connections between different brain regions. Two strongly connected brain areas, for example, might be expected to have a tendency to light up during the same tasks, one just a fraction of a second after the other. Such approaches have led to the identification of a number of brain circuits and have also shown that precise properties of the brain circuits or networks vary between people. These variations can, to some extent, be inherited, and seem to be related to the risk of some mental disorders.
this type of thing) rather then rely on old discredited clinical judgement to define the disorders. Meanwhile the tendency to label disorders as a substitute for understanding, and to mistake a label as understanding, is going to be a hard habit to break. This is a shame as the stigma of the diagnosis of mental illness is probably as prevalent and lamentable in most parts of the world now as it was in 1973 when Rosenhan was writing. His article - lucid, humane, and trenchantly honest - is worth revisiting:
“we tend to invent "knowledge" and assume that we understand more than we actually do. We seem unable to acknowledge that we simply don't know. The needs for diagnosis and remediation of behavioral and emotional problems are enormous. But rather than acknowledge that we are just embarking on understanding, we continue to label patients "schizophrenic," "manic-depressive," and "insane," as if in those words we had captured the essence of understanding. The facts of the matter are that we have known for a long time that diagnoses are often not useful or reliable, but we have nevertheless continued to use them.”
References
[1] Ronson J (2011). The Psychopath Test.
[2] Rosenhan DL (1973). On being sane in insane places. Science 179; 250-8.
[3] Frances A (2012). Diagnosing the D.S.M. N Y Times, May 12, A19.
[4] Buckholtz JW, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2012). Psychopathology and the human connectome: toward a transdiagnostic model of risk for mental illness. Neuron 74; 990-1004.
Monday 25 June 2012
Alan Turing, Autism & Ice Cream
On 28th May 2012 my ice-cream obsessed affectionate musical happy empathetic party-loving mischievous expressive easy-going popular curious and undeniably quizzical three year old son (let’s call him Joseph K) was assessed by a trio of clip-board wielding psychosomethings. After one hour of observation and a short break for deliberation they reconvened and pronounced the sentence. “Autism”. On the 29th May I visited Bletchley Park with a small group including my 85 year old father who had trained as a radio operator there during the second world war when it was the centre of British code-breaking operations. He was keen to visit the hut where he had worked, and was excited to discover that the adjacent building had held the spartan office of an athletic playful romantic thoughtful witty humane driven and undeniably eccentric mathematician and cryptanalyst named Alan Turing. During and shortly after the war, Turing’s name was almost unknown outside the worlds of mathematics, cryptanalysis and long-distance running. But in mathematics he had achieved distinction in his early 20s by showing 1) why the world was predictable in its unpredictability; and 2) that its unknowability could be known. The former required proving the central limit theorem, which explains why many traits, such as IQs, tend to follow a bell-shaped or Gaussian curve (Turing later discovered that a Finnish mathematician, Jarl Lindeberg, had proved the result 13 years earlier). The latter involved solving the meaty Entscheidungsproblem, the decision problem, by showing (decisively) that there was no solution: no algorithm exists for deciding whether a statement of first order logic is valid based on the axioms and rules of logic. This time he was pipped by just a few weeks by the American logician Alonzo Church. Some people (I’m not one of them) think this work has major implications for understanding consciousness. Turing also had trials for a place in the British marathon team for the 1948 London Olympics, but failed to qualify by just a few minutes.
Exactly 100 years after his birth, Turing is now (of course) recognised to have made some of the most original and far-reaching contributions to mathematics, computing, theoretical biology, logic and cryptanalysis. In his own time the mind that made these contributions was thought by the psychiatric profession to be diseased. Turing enjoyed having sex with other men. Until 1973 homosexuality was listed as a mental disorder in the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic Manual (DSM). A related diagnosis “ego-dystonic homosexuality” (the impressive sounding name for “persistent distress from a sustained pattern of unwanted homosexual arousal” or “a persistent lack of heterosexual arousal, which the patient experienced as interfering with initiation or maintenance of wanted heterosexual relationships”) didn’t get kicked out until 1986. Which is a roundabout way of saying there might be one or two reasons for not taking the DSM too seriously.
Strawberry deprivation
We live in enlightened times. Turing, following his conviction for homosexual conduct in 1952, was given a choice between prison and forced injections of female hormones. My “autistic” son can, theoretically, choose from a smorgasbord of non-pharamaceutical “early intervention” programs and (on the paediatrician’s recommendations) between a gluten-free diet and strawberry deprivation therapy. One or two of these interventions might even be supported by evidence of their effectiveness. But how enlightened will 2012 look in 60 years time? I would be surprised if we will be able to look back without being slightly appalled both by the crudity of our classifications of different types of mind, and at the inhumanity we still tolerate towards those whose minds work differently. For many classed has having mental disorders, such as Turing, the only suffering the "disorder" has caused is due to the bullying or thoughtlessness of others. But what kind of disorder is that? If it’s the bullies who are causing the suffering why don’t we decide that they are they are the group with the mental disorder and treat them appropriately?
Thinking machines
Alan Turing thought that brains were matter and could be studied and understood just as machines can be studied and understood. I agree. As the neurologically-intriguing nature of my son has revealed itself an exploration of various areas of brain science research has become something of a hobby. In a sense, this research is a global attempt to decipher the ultimate cryptogram, and is as exciting and glamorous an enterprise as the code-cracking at Bletchley. I plan to blog intermittently about aspects of this research - and in particular about language, music, dreams, autism, Williams Syndrome, and other brain-related subjects from the perspective of a curious and interested outsider. I am a practicing scientist and work in the fields of epidemiology and mathematical biology, another area where Alan Turing made a wonderful contribution [1]. I know next to nothing about brains, but I do know something about how to read a scientific paper, and I hope this will help me to make some interesting research accessible to a wider public. There are also some great experiments the brain hobbyist can do at home, both on his or her own brain or on the brains of friends, lovers, parents, children. I'll try to collect some of those here.
On the visit to Bletchley my son became fascinated by the working model of a bombe, Turing’s stupendously complex code-breaking machine. Turing's bombe had evolved from a machine developed by Polish cryptanalysts which was whimsically named after the ice cream sundae - bomba or bombe - they were eating when they came up with the idea for the machine (some obsessions are universal). Stupendously complex, that is, until you see the working model of the Colossus round the corner, and then realise that that monster contains only a tiny fraction of the computing power of the phone in your pocket, which in turn is a computational nematode compared to cognitive leviathan that has just finished decoding this sentence.
[1] The Chemical Basis of Morphogenesis (1952). Turing AM. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, Vol. 237, No. 641, pp. 37-72.
http://www.dna.caltech.edu/courses/cs191/paperscs191/turing.pdf
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)